Thursday, August 1, 2019
Narratives in Conversation
Narratives in Conversation By Agatha Xaris Villa INTRODUCTION This essay focuses on the study of the narrative most prevalent in everyday conversations ââ¬â the conversational narrative. First, it discusses a definition of the narrative from a structural level based on the structure of conversational narrative presented by William Labov (1972). Next, it enumerates some of the important functions which the narrative is able to achieve both on a personal level and also on the interpersonal. Lastly, it ventures to explore the notion of the narrative based on its context ââ¬â language and society, culture.NARRATIVES: A STRUCTURAL PERSPECTIVE In the 1960s and 1970s, William Labov developed what is now commonly believed to be the general structure of a narrative. His research involved ââ¬Ëa focus on spontaneous recounting of experienceââ¬â¢ and the capturing of vernacular, unmonitored speech. He interviewed African American youths in South Harlem and asked them whether they h ad ever encountered a life-endangering experience. He found that the stories which ensued from these interviews ââ¬Ëreduced the effects of observation to a minimumââ¬â¢ (Labov, 2001) and referred to them as oral narratives of personal experience.Through his study, Labov noted some very important structural characteristics of oral narratives. First, he observed that the events featured in narratives often appeared in the order in which they actually happened. According to his definition, the narrative was a way of retelling (i. e. narrating) the action sequence of an event that had already happened. Therefore, parts of conversation considered to be ââ¬Ënarrativeââ¬â¢ was limited to the discursive data contributing to the recounting of the turn of events.All other parts which were not directly related to the story served the purpose of backing up the story. He claimed that these oral narratives usually had a basic structure composed of any of the following six basic parts: (1) Abstract, (2) Orientation, (3) Complicating action, (4) Evaluation, (5) Resolution and (6) Coda. Using a transcript of ââ¬ËSample Stories ââ¬â Stories about mothersââ¬â¢ (CD-ROM 1, Band 6)(Appendix 1. 1), we may illustrate these elements as they occur in actual oral narratives. The data presented has two speakers: a female interviewer (A) and a male interviewee (B).The transcript also features two accounts: the first is with regards to (B)ââ¬â¢s early memories of his mother who was training as an educational psychologist and the second of which occurred sometime during his adolescence. By definition, an abstract is a synopsis of what the story is about. Looking at the transcript of ââ¬ËStories about mothersââ¬â¢, it is useful to note that the first story offered by (B) seems to lack an abstract. This, however, is understandable as his story was elicited by (A)ââ¬â¢s question regarding childhood memories of his mother. Therefore, the interviewer provided th e abstract prior to the beginning of the narrative.In the second story, the abstract is located in line 21-22 wherein (B) cites when his mother was ââ¬Ëmost sympatheticââ¬â¢ to him ââ¬â during his ââ¬Ëadolescent rebellion stageââ¬â¢. Typically, the orientation appears first in the narration and begins by citing the basic details of the story: that is, the ââ¬Ëwhoââ¬â¢, the ââ¬Ëwhatââ¬â¢, ââ¬Ëwhereââ¬â¢ and ââ¬Ëwhat were they doingââ¬â¢ of the narrative. In our transcript we can detect the beginning of an orientation in lines 5-10 in which (B) recounts when his mother had begun studying educational psychology and some of the tests she carried out on him and his brother.The complicating action effectively answers the question ââ¬â ââ¬Ëthen, what happened? ââ¬â¢ An example may be found in line 11 where (B) said recalls his motherââ¬â¢s reaction to a ââ¬Ëslightly alarmingââ¬â¢ result of an inkblot test. The complicating action i s the only element which Labov believed to be a pre-requisite to a narrative. On the other hand, evaluation is the only element which does not necessarily happen in sequential order in a story. It may occur in any part of the story, most noticeably at the storyââ¬â¢s climax and answers the question ââ¬â ââ¬Ëso what? ââ¬â¢.It may come in the form of ââ¬Ëexternal evaluationââ¬â¢ (narrator steps in to give an evaluation)(e. g. line 10: referring to the intelligence tests as ââ¬Ëactually fineââ¬â¢) or ââ¬Ëinternal evaluationââ¬â¢ such as intensifiers (which emphasize on aspects of a story)(e. g. ââ¬Ëâ⬠¦she took it very, very seriouslyââ¬â¢. ), paralinguistic features (e. g. the use of sound effects, facial expressions and gestures) and the taking on of other peopleââ¬â¢s voices (Bakhtin, 1981). Evaluation is important to the narrative as it is a way for narrators to emphasize important parts of the story by drawing the attention of his listene rs by adding more information.An obvious paralinguistic feature which we can deduce from the transcript was (B)ââ¬â¢s use of tone and pause. At about line 11, he starts using a rather loud and fast-paced voice but the dynamic changes in line 13 before (B) voices out his mother saying ââ¬ËOh I seeâ⬠¦Ã¢â¬â¢ followed by a pregnant pause for ââ¬Ëdramatic effectââ¬â¢. It may be argued that tone and the pauses were used evaluatively and therefore is a very good example of how people are able to use language creatively even in everyday conversation. In a narrative, the resolution is what we refer to as the concluding action ââ¬â what finally happened.For example, in conclusion to the memory of the slight scare regarding the inkblot test, we are told that (B)ââ¬â¢s mother reassures him that he neednââ¬â¢t worry because she was ââ¬Ëjust learningââ¬â¢ (line 15-16). The final element, the Coda, tells us how the story is relevant to the storyteller or his audie nce in the present time. A potential coda may be found in line 18 where (B) jokes that he now thinks that the results of his motherââ¬â¢s tests were ââ¬Ërightââ¬â¢. The Labovian categories regarding the narrative structure has been criticized for ââ¬Ëconstrictingââ¬â¢ what can be considered a story but continues to have significant impact on narrative study even today.It persists to be a useful procedure in narrative analysis by providing a succinct set of categories for defining ââ¬Ëstoriesââ¬â¢. Furthermore, while it emphasizes on the ââ¬Ëflowââ¬â¢ relationship between ââ¬Ëexperience, cognition and representationââ¬â¢ ââ¬â it spends a fair amount of time taking the language into consideration before attempting to make sense of it (Squire, 2008). NARRATIVES: A FUNCTIONAL PERSPECTIVE Rather than identifying narratives according to their structural characteristics, we may also define narratives according to the function they accomplish in relation to the storyteller and his or her audience.In the process of storytelling, the narrator is not only able to recount the happenings of past events but in addition, the resultant narrative is imbued with select aspects of the tellerââ¬â¢s identity. Through the narrative, the storyteller tells his audience the ââ¬Ëkind of personââ¬â¢ he sees himself as. (Toolan, 2006, p. 63). Narrative identity may be projected in the narratorââ¬â¢s use of evaluative language. Sometimes during the course of an oral narrative, the speaker might use different styles (style-shifting) or language varieties (i. . codeswitching) in order to ââ¬Ëcreate voicesââ¬â¢ for their characters and may serve to help in the transmission of identity and allegiance (Maybin, 1996). In ââ¬ËStories about Mothersââ¬â¢, (B) portrayed himself as an active participant in his motherââ¬â¢s tests. In retrospect, he seems to be telling us that at present, he is rather fond of his childhood memories of his mother (perhaps alluding that he has a good relationship with his mother now) and that he has since grown out of his rebellious teenage stage (heââ¬â¢s ââ¬Ëmatureââ¬â¢ now).Narratives not only serve a personal function but may also serve an important interpersonal purpose. According to the work of Norrick on nuclear families (1997), participation in co-narration (which is common practice in families and others in close relationships) is a way for family members to demonstrate and therefore solidify their position within the family. He believes that family membership may be measured by the contribution that a member is able to provide to the act of shared narration. Through the shared arration of past or recurring events, individuals exhibit shared group identity and allegiance. Norrick also points out that shared narration may also function as a way to demonstrate shared values within families. While an outsider may not have the advantage of having shared experiences with other family members and so be unable to participate in the co-telling of past events, he or she may use his or her own experiences which are similar to those of the family in order, thereby adopting values inherent with the family.In this way, outsiders may gain acceptance as ââ¬Ëinsidersââ¬â¢ of the family. All this is in agreement with Brunerââ¬â¢s suggestion that ââ¬Ëour sensitivity to narrative provides the major link between our own sense of self and our sense of others in the social world around usââ¬â¢ (1986). NARRATIVES: A CONTEXTUAL PERSPECTIVE In order to gain a full understanding of the narrative, one must take context into serious consideration because stories are constantly under the influence of the narratorââ¬â¢s representation of experience as well as of language, society and culture.While it is a narratorââ¬â¢s prerogative to portray the characters and events of his story in a manner of his choice, the details of the story and the way that it i s told is largely influenced by the storytellerââ¬â¢s personal notions of himself, those around him and events. Stories, whether they are in written or oral form are all representations of the narratorââ¬â¢s experience (Toolan, 2006, p. 68). External and internal evaluations are important tools used by the narrator to be able to represent their experience of people and events through the narrative.In addition, evaluation may also be ââ¬Ëembeddedââ¬â¢ into narratives when narrators directly quote a character (Toolan, 2006, p. 68). In ââ¬ËStories about mothersââ¬â¢, [B] repeatedly gives voice to his mother by quoting some of the things she used to say (e. g. Youââ¬â¢ll be fine! ). How experiences are represented all depends on the narratorââ¬â¢s personal constructs. Personal constructs are inner worlds and perceptions that determine the way that we ââ¬Ëseeââ¬â¢ people and events and are largely determined and affected by language, culture and society.The importance of taking the socio-cultural context of a narrative is important especially when we move to decoding the ââ¬Ëmeaningââ¬â¢ behind narratives. In 1975, Grice explained that everyday conversations were being guided by four maxims: the maxims of Quality, Quantity, Relation and Manner (Toolan, 2006, 72). These maxims (inferential norms or so called conversation implicatures) explain how individuals are able to ââ¬Ëlogicallyââ¬â¢ comprehend meaning in conversation but may be insufficient when trying to explain how stories or jokes are understood.Nair (2002) proposed that in the process of narrative inferencing (i. e. , the way that we figure out what a story means), cultural cooperation between the narrator and the audience is of utmost importance. The implications regarding the importance of impliculture in narrative inferencing is that we are reminded that (1) there are aspects of narratives which are culturally situated and must be interpreted in the context of t he same culture and (2) therefore, only individuals who share the same cultural identity may fully interpret cultural narratives.The relationship between the narrative and culture is further demonstrated by the fact that stories of folklore and native traditions have been passed on from generation to generation in the form of the narrative. In this way, narratives contribute to the preservation and transmission of cultural identity and belonging. CONCLUSION Stories are commonplace in everyday life. In fact, it has been said that conversational English is largely in the form of the narrative (Maybin, 1996) ââ¬â in laidback conversations with friends and relatives to our more professional interactions with colleagues in the workplace.They can be identified through their structural characteristics, the personal and interpersonal functions they serve in everyday social life and are both the result and inferred from the socio-cultural context and yet, the oral narrative remains a nat ural part of human life from the cradle to the grave ââ¬â from the silly bed time stories we are told in childhood, to the epitaphs told in ââ¬Ëour memoryââ¬â¢ at our lives end. REFERENCES Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The Dialogic Imagination. Austin. State University of Texas Press. Bruner, J. 1986). Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, Mass. Harvard University Press. Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. and Morgan, J. (eds) Syntax and Semantics. 3: Speech Acts. New York: Academic Press. Labov, W. (1972). Language in the Inner City. Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania Press. Labov, W. (2001). The Social Stratification of English in New York City. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Maybin, J. (1996)(Ed). Chapter 1 ââ¬ËEveryday Talkââ¬â¢. In Maybin, J. and Mercer, N. eds) Using English from conversation to canon. Routledge/The Open University. pg. 21-27 Nair, R. B. (2002). Narrative Gravity. Chapter 5 ââ¬ËRationality and rel evanceââ¬â¢. Oxford, Oxford University Press. Norrick, N. R. (1997). Language in Society, 26(2). pg. 199-220. Squire, C. (2008). Reading Narratives. http://www. uel. ac. uk/cnr/documents/CNRWIPJune04Squire. doc (accessed: July 2008) Toolan, M. (2006). Chapter 2 ââ¬ËTelling Storiesââ¬â¢. In Maybin, J. and Swann, J. (eds) The art of English: everyday creativity. The Open University. pg. 54-76
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.